|
Programming for Learning Across Restricted-and Free-Operant Arrangements: What We’ve Learned in Basic, Translational, Applied, and Professional Service Explorations |
Sunday, May 25, 2025 |
10:00 AM–11:50 AM |
Convention Center, Street Level, 147 A |
Area: EDC/EAB; Domain: Applied Research |
Chair: Jennifer Wertalik (Georgia Southern University- Armstrong) |
Discussant: Andrew Bulla (Georgia Southern University - Armstrong ) |
CE Instructor: Andrew Bulla, Ph.D. |
Abstract: Researchers in the field of behavior analysis and precision teaching have long had discussions about the difference between free-operant and discrete-trial performance. Many of these discussions have centered around criticisms of both types of approaches for establishing new behaviors in both human and non-human organisms. Limited research exists comparing what differences exist, if any, between teaching new behaviors under restricted conditions (e.g., discrete trial) and freer conditions (e.g., frequency building). The first talk will discuss a series of basic experimental studies using non-human models (rattus) to isolate a series of variables in understanding the benefits of acquiring novel discriminations under both free- and restricted-operant paradigms. The second presentation will extend what we learned in the lab to a translational paradigm comparing both conditioning procedures to teach numerals in non-native languages. The third presentation will then demonstrate how findings from basic and translational arrangements to inform an applied behavior analytic approach to teaching correct Spanish pronunciation to non-native Spanish speakers. Lastly, the fourth presentation will describe how behavior analysts can use these data to program for learning in professional practice. We will then discuss how we can use the findings of these evaluations across the four arrangements to inform the professional practice of behavior analysis. |
Instruction Level: Basic |
Keyword(s): Discrete Trial, EAB, Free Operant, Restricted Operant |
Target Audience: The target audience is behavior analysts of all kinds. We will show the full range of basic, translational, applied, and the professional practice of behavior analysis, all investigating the same research question. |
Learning Objectives: 1. State and give examples of restricted operant arrangements 2. State and give examples of free operant arrangements 3. Discriminate which of their current programs represent restricted or free operant approahces |
|
The Effects of Free-Operant and Restricted-Operant Procedures on the Acquisition, Maintenance, and Stability of Novel Discriminations in Rats |
SODEEQ ADESANYA (University of Nebraska Medical Center), Andrew Bulla (Georgia Southern University - Armstrong), Jennifer Wertalik (Georgia Southern University- Armstrong), Madisen Duke (Georgia Southern University - Armstrong), Kaitlyn Wyllie (Georgia Southern University - Armstrong), Leah Yakabovits (Georgia Southern University), Amanda S Bradley (Georgia Southern University - Armstrong), Laura Catalina Micán (Georgia Southern University) |
Abstract: Two common teaching paradigms exist in which one can measure and assess the acquisition and maintenance of learning: Free-operant and restricted-operant (sometimes referred to as discrete trial) procedures. In a free-operant paradigm, the organism freely makes a response at any given time with no restrictions imposed upon them (Hachiya & Masato, 1991). In a restricted-operant paradigm, the organism has restrictions placed upon them and they cannot respond freely; the organism must wait for an external stimulus before they can respond (Hachiya & Masato, 1991). We investigated whether one specific teaching paradigm will lead to better stability (i.e., persistence in the face of distractions), maintenance (i.e., same levels of performance after a period of no practice), and performance of discrimination tasks (i.e., acquisition of the new response). We conducted a preliminary study to identify the specific methodologies researcher should use to identify the effects of the two teaching paradigms while controlling three distinct variables: (1) Time in the teaching environment, (2) The number of responses emitted in both conditions, and (3) the number of reinforcers delivered in each condition. |
|
Comparing Acquisition and Fluency Outcomes Under Restricted and Free Operant Teaching Methods: A Systematic Replication |
AMANDA S BRADLEY (Georgia Southern University - Armstrong), Andrew Bulla (Georgia Southern University - Armstrong), Jennifer Wertalik (Georgia Southern University- Armstrong) |
Abstract: Discrete-trial teaching (DTT), a restricted-operant teaching arrangement, and frequency-building instruction (FBI), a free-operant teaching arrangement, represent two instructional strategies derived from operant conditioning. Researchers and practicing behavior analysts have used both to establish and firm up novel stimulus-behavior relations. Despite the effectiveness of both procedures, few studies have compared the two techniques and assessed the effects on the emergence of fluent responding. The current study extends the research to typically developing college students to directly compare DTT and FBI. We taught participants the numerals 0-10 in unknown foreign languages (i.e., Mandarin, Arabic, and Hindi) using both procedures. Under both conditions, we held constant the number of practice trials and frequency of reinforcement. We replicated Bulla et al. (2024) by also controlled the size of the instructional material, and tested for maintenance of performance. We discuss the results of the study in the context of planning for learning across three stages of learning. |
|
Integration of Video Modeling and TAGteach With Precision Teaching to Improve Articulation in Non-native Spanish Speakers |
LAURA CATALINA MICÁN (Georgia Southern University), Andrew Bulla (Georgia Southern University - Armstrong), Encarnación Cruz Jiménez (Georgia Southern University - Armstrong) |
Abstract: A learner’s native language impacts the acquisition of a second language (Moreno-Duron, 2019; Broad, 2020). Learners’ habits in their first language hinder the development of habits to learn a second language (Broad, 2020). The current study used the principles of instructional design and behavior analysis to help improve the articulation of specific sounds in Spanish to non-native Spanish speakers. Using a multiple baseline design, we evaluated a sequence of instructional strategies on the application to novel Spanish words. In the first phase, individuals discriminated between video examples and non-examples of correctly pronounced Spanish words in a restricted-operant (RO) paradigm. In phase two, the learners watched a video model of the correct presentation, and attempted to copy the model. We used TAGteach to reinforce correct pronunciation in an RO paradigm. In phase three, learners then built frequencies on non-sense Spanish words that contained the target sounds in a free-operant arrangement. We collected data on the number of correctly pronounced non-sense Spanish words, as well as measured the application of the trained skills to novel words in third-grade-level stories using the Spanish version of the Dynamic Indicators of Basic Early Literacy Skills (DIBELS). |
|
Freeing the Operant in Large Scale Education Settings: Morningside’s Three Phases of Teaching and Learning |
ANDREW ROBERT KIETA (Morningside Academy, The Wing Institute at Morningside Academy), Kent Johnson (Morningside Academy) |
Abstract: The concept of a restricted to free operant paradigm underlies the design of the three phases of teaching and learning in the Morningside Model of Generative Instruction: Initial instruction, practice to fluency with celeration using Precision Teaching, and application/simple generative responding to real-world environments. In each of these three phases, instructional designers and teachers systematically design learning materials and contexts such that they shift from restricted to free operant. Initial instruction is necessarily restricted, as students only respond to the external stimuli presented by the teacher. Teachers fade out the frequency of those external stimuli until students respond in a less restricted fashion as they near accuracy. Then, free operant thinking is applied to the design of Precision Teaching materials, as students are given the opportunity to respond as frequently as they can to achieve outcomes associated with fluency. Finally, new restrictions are established as students are taught how, when, and why to apply fluent behaviors to real-world contexts, with those restrictions faded out in favor of more free operant arrangements, permitting students to apply fluent behavior wherever it is possible and likely to be followed by reinforcement. |
|
|